Review article
Effect of acute hypoxia on cognition: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.019Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Hypoxia has a negative effect on cognition.

  • Effect on central executive and non-executive tasks did not differ significantly.

  • Low (<60 mmHg) arterial partial pressure of oxygen predicts poor cognition.

  • Hypobaric versus normobaric hypoxic conditions was not a significant moderator.

Abstract

A systematic meta-regression analysis of the effects of acute hypoxia on the performance of central executive and non-executive tasks, and the effects of the moderating variables, arterial partial pressure of oxygen (PaO2) and hypobaric versus normobaric hypoxia, was undertaken. Studies were included if they were performed on healthy humans; within-subject design was used; data were reported giving the PaO2 or that allowed the PaO2 to be estimated (e.g. arterial oxygen saturation and/or altitude); and the duration of being in a hypoxic state prior to cognitive testing was ≤6 days. Twenty-two experiments met the criteria for inclusion and demonstrated a moderate, negative mean effect size (g = −0.49, 95% CI −0.64 to −0.34, p < 0.001). There were no significant differences between central executive and non-executive, perception/attention and short-term memory, tasks. Low (35–60 mmHg) PaO2 was the key predictor of cognitive performance (R2 = 0.45, p < 0.001) and this was independent of whether the exposure was in hypobaric hypoxic or normobaric hypoxic conditions.

Introduction

The military, mountain rescuers, mountaineers and many other individuals, are required to work and live at high altitudes. With increasing altitude, the barometric pressure decreases exponentially, resulting in a progressive reduction in the ambient partial pressure of oxygen (PO2), termed hypobaric hypoxia. For practical and logistical reasons, normobaric hypoxia is often used as a laboratory alternative to hypobaric hypoxia, whereby the inspired oxygen fraction is reduced to account for the greater barometric pressure and elicit an ‘altitude-equivalent’ lowering of PO2 (Conkin, 2011). An underlying assumption with this isohypoxia approach is that PO2 is the only relevant physiological stimulus, but there is some evidence for physiological differences elicited by hypobaric hypoxia compared to the isohypoxic, normobaric equivalent (Coppel et al., 2015, Normand and Koehle, 2012). Nevertheless, both approaches reduce the slope of the oxygen transport cascade from the atmosphere to the mitochondria, eliciting manifold physiological effects resulting primarily from a lower arterial PO2 (PaO2) and reduced oxyhemoglobin saturation (Marconi and Cerretelli, 2008) The precise nature of the response to hypoxic environments is influenced by the magnitude of the stimulus: altitudes up to ∼2000–2500 m are in the flat portion of the sigmoidal oxyhemoglobin dissociation curve, whereas higher altitudes are in the steep portion of the curve and require more pronounced adjustment (Lundby et al., 2008). However, broadly speaking, the initial responses to altitude exposure serve to maintain oxygen supply. Hypoxic stimulation of the carotid bodies increases alveolar ventilation, causing respiratory alkalosis (Marconi and Cerretelli, 2008), and augments sympathoadrenal activity, increasing peripheral epinephrine levels (Epi) (Mazzeo and Reeves, 2013), heart rate and cardiac output (Kahler et al., 1962); while peripheral norepinephrine (NE) levels may progressively increase over the initial six-day exposure (Mazzeo and Reeves, 2003).

Within the first hours of exposure, plasma volume also decreases, possibly due to redistribution of fluid from the extra- to intra-cellular fluid compartment (Hannon et al., 1969). Although this reduces total blood volume, red cell volume is unchanged and the oxygen carrying capacity per unit of blood is increased thus augmenting the oxygen delivery for a given cardiac output. Although, in this study, we concentrate on acute hypoxia (≤ 6 days), we should note that with chronic hypoxic exposure (acclimatization) the plasma volume is restored and stimulation of erythopoeisis increases the number of erythrocytes (Pugh, 1964), which, in combination with an increased arterio-venous oxygen difference, enables a reduced cardiac output for a given metabolic oxygen demand (Wolfel et al., 1998). Nevertheless, with both acute and chronic hypoxia, the performance of physical work requiring high rates of aerobic metabolism is impaired, relative to the normoxic work capacity (Pugh, 1967), although this decrement may be lower with normobaric than hypobaric hypoxia (Saugy et al., 2016) and is partially attenuated with acclimation and acclimatization (Pugh, 1967).

While the effects of acute hypoxia on physical performance have been studied extensively, there is comparatively little research into the effects on cognitive skills, such as visual search and decision making. These skills typically require attention, perception, executive functioning and short-term memory (STM). Moreover, few authors have attempted to review the work and, to the best of our knowledge, nobody has sought to systematically review this area using meta-analytical methods. Recently, Taylor and colleagues (2016) completed a narrative review and demonstrated a tendency towards inhibition of cognition by acute hypoxia, however these findings were equivocal and inconclusive. In a review focusing primarily on clinical neuropsychological measures, Virués-Ortega et al. (2004) showed a tendency for acute hypoxia to induce decrements in psychophysiological measures, e.g. P300 latency and amplitude, but this was not always manifest in outcome measures, e.g. reaction time. Although the aforementioned, narrative reviews were unable to provide definitive conclusions, both groups of authors observed similar tendencies, with central executive tasks demonstrating negative effects while the non-executive, perception/attention and short-term memory (STM) tasks showed limited effects. This is in line with studies examining the effects of acute exercise (McMorris and Hale, 2012), heat (Cian et al., 2001, McMorris et al., 2006a) and sleep deprivation (McMorris et al., 2006b) on cognitive function. The findings of Taylor et al. and Virués-Ortega et al. also provide some support for lower PaO2 resulting in greater inhibition of performance than more moderate levels of PaO2 (readers not familiar with PaO2 should note that lower PaO2 means a greater negative effect of hypoxia than moderate levels of PaO2). Observation of the studies reviewed by these authors also showed that some studies examined the effect of normobaric hypoxia while others utilized hypobaric hypoxia. Research has suggested that the two conditions may well have different effects on stress due to their differing environmental conditions (Coppel et al., 2015). To summarize the conclusions of Taylor et al. and Virués-Ortega et al., we could say that the empirical literature reviewed provided little strong evidence for a significant effect of hypoxia on cognition but the trend is for an inhibitory effect, especially at low levels of PaO2 and mainly for central executive tasks.

Given that cognition requires oxygen activation at every stage (Virués-Ortega et al., 2004), one might expect hypoxia to have a resounding negative effect and that the failure of the narrative reviews to demonstrate this unequivocally is counterintuitive. However, animal studies have shown that when PaO2 falls below ∼60 mmHg, chemoreceptors in the carotid body sense the fall and feedback, via the glossypharyngeal nerve, to the nucleus tractus solitarii (NTS), where they activate tyrosine hydroxylase (TH)-containing catecholaminergic neurons. The NTS projects to the ventrolateral medulla (VLM) (Guyenet et al., 2013) and the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (King et al., 2013, Rinaman, 2011), regions important in the control of autonomic functions. This results in the release of the catecholamine neurotransmitters NE and Epi. Moreover, catecholaminergic neurons also project to the locus coeruleus (LC) (Abbott et al., 2012, Guyenet et al., 2013), which is the main source of NE in the brain. Release of NE has been shown to increase Ca2+ signaling in astrocytes, which is associated with the release of vasodilatory astroglial messengers; dilatation of brain microvessels; and, hence, increases in cerebral blood flow (CBF) (Toussay et al., 2013). Similarly, during hypoxia, feedback to the NTS from visceral afferents and carotid body arterial chemoreceptors has been shown to activate non-TH-containing neurons. These non-catecholaminergic neurons project to the rostral VLM (Guyenet et al., 2013) and, also, stimulate the brain’s response to hypoxia. Moreover, adenosine, which is released from the carotid body during hypoxia, plays a role in increasing CBF by stimulating the release of nitric oxide (NO) from vascular endothelium vessels (Ray et al., 2002). NO, mediated by its second messenger cyclic guanosine monophosphate, plays a major role in vasodilation during hypoxia (Umbrello et al., 2012). These hypoxia-induced increases in CBF may account for the apparent disparity between the empirical research results reviewed by Taylor et al. (2016) and Virués-Ortega et al. and what one would expect based on the importance of oxygen during cognition and the lack of it during hypoxia. In other words, increased CBF during hypoxia compensates for lower PaO2. However, several authors have questioned the ability of increases in hypoxia-induced CBF to ensure a sufficient supply of oxygen for proficient performance of many tasks, including cognitive functioning (Binks et al., 2008, Ogoh et al., 2013, Ogoh et al., 2014).

Examination of the results of the studies reviewed by Taylor et al. (2016) and Virués-Ortega et al. (2004) also raises questions concerning the ability of hypoxia-induced increased CBF to ensure maintenance of cognitive performance. Moreover, that many of the studies reviewed had small sample sizes leads one to question their power and it is distinctly possible that, at least, some of these studies displayed Type II errors, which hid a significant deterioration in cognition. We, therefore, decided to carry out a systematic meta-regression analysis, which places the emphasis on effect sizes rather than probability levels, thus compensating for low power. It also allows us to examine the effects of potential modulators on the findings. As a result, firstly, we undertook a test to determine the mean effect size for the effects of acute hypoxia on cognition. Based on the literature, outlined above, concerning increased CBF and the results of the narrative reviews, we hypothesized a significant, main effect of hypoxia on cognition, with a negative mean effect size being demonstrated. Similarly, given that both sets of reviewers argued that results showed a trend for an effect of task type and that research into stress, in general, on animals demonstrates such an effect (see Arnsten, 2009, Arnsten, 2011), our second hypothesis was that central executive tasks would be significantly more negatively affected than non-executive, perception/attention and STM tasks. Our third hypothesis was that low PaO2 would predict a larger, negative mean effect size than moderate PaO2. This was based on the fact that the level for moderate hypoxia, which we designated for this study, might not induce feedback by the carotid body to the NTS (Virués-Ortega et al., 2004, West, 2004) and, hence, alter neurotransmitter activity in the brain. Finally, we hypothesized that hypobaric hypoxia would predict poorer cognitive functioning than during normobaric conditions. We also decided to examine the possibility of an interaction effect between PaO2 level and normobaric/hypobaric conditions.

Section snippets

Method

A systematic literature search, using the following data bases, Pubmed, SCOPUS, SportsDISCUS and Web of Knowledge, was undertaken. Each database was searched from their earliest available record up to September 2016. Key words used in the searches were combinations of “altitude”; “attention”; “central executive”; “cognition”; “hypobaric”; “hypoxia”; “learning”; “long-term memory”; “normobaric”; “perception”; “short-term memory” and “working memory”; In addition; reference lists from empirical

Included studies

The characteristics of the included studies can be seen in Table 1. The literature reviewed yielded 68 articles which examined hypoxia and cognition. Of these, 18 met the criteria for inclusion. Four of these articles reported two experiments using different participants in each experiment, therefore these were treated as separate studies, taking the total number of experiments examined to 22. Sixteen experiments included only one task type, while six included two task types. Mean effect sizes

Discussion

This is the first review to systematically examine the effect of acute hypoxia on central executive, perception/attention and STM tasks, and the effect of the moderating variables, low versus moderate PaO2 and normobaric versus hypobaric conditions, and the interaction between the two variables. The results of this meta-analysis supply only limited support for our hypotheses. Firstly, as hypothesized, the main effect showed a moderate, negative mean effect size and is evidence for a

Conclusion

In conclusion, the key findings to emerge from this review are a) hypoxia has a negative effect on cognition, b) this is regardless of whether the task is central executive or a non-executive perception/attention or STM task, and c) it is likely that PaO2 level, and not whether the exposure is in hypobaric hypoxic or normobaric hypoxic conditions, is the key predictor of cognitive performance.

References1 (88)

  • G.E. Gibson et al.

    Brain dysfunction in mild to moderate hypoxia

    Am. J. Med.

    (1981)
  • R. *Hayashi et al.

    Effects of simulated high altitude on event-related potential (P300) and auditory brain-stem responses

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2005)
  • T. Hemmingsson et al.

    Lower exhaled nitric oxide in hypobaric than in normobaric acute hypoxia

    Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol.

    (2009)
  • T. *Komiyama et al.

    Does moderate hypoxia alter working memory and executive function during prolonged exercise?

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2015)
  • P. *Li et al.

    Training-dependent cognitive advantage is suppressed at high altitude

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2012)
  • T. McMorris et al.

    Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition: a meta-analytical investigation

    Brain Cogn.

    (2012)
  • T. McMorris et al.

    Heat stress, plasma concentrations of epinephrine, norepinephrine, 5-hydroxytryptamine and cortisol: perception of mood state and working memory

    Int. J. Psychophysiol.

    (2006)
  • T. McMorris

    Developing the catecholamines hypothesis for the acute exercise-cognition interaction in humans: lessons from animal studies

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2016)
  • A. Miyake et al.

    The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks: a latent variable analysis

    Cogn. Psychol.

    (2000)
  • S.F. Rimoldi et al.

    Acute and chronic altitude-induced cognitive dysfunction in children and adolescents

    J. Pediatr.

    (2016)
  • B. Shukitt-Hale et al.

    Nimodipine prevents the in vivo decrease in hippocampal extracellular acetylcholine produced by hypobaric hypoxia

    Brain Res.

    (1993)
  • N. *Tsarouchas et al.

    Effects of moderate hypobaric hypoxia on evoked categorical visuocognitive responses

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2008)
  • H.-Y. Wang et al.

    Muscarinic modulation of hypoxia-induced release of catecholamines from the cat carotid body

    Brain Res.

    (2002)
  • B.D. Waterhouse et al.

    Noradrenergic modulation of somatosensory cortical neuronal responses to iontophoretically applied putative transmitters

    Exp. Neurol.

    (1980)
  • B.D. Waterhouse et al.

    Alpha-receptor-mediated facilitation of somatosensory cortical neuronal responses to excitatory synaptic inputs and iontophoretically applied acetylcholine

    Neuropharmacol

    (1981)
  • S.B. Abbott et al.

    C1 neurons excite locus coeruleus and A5 noradrenergic neurons along with sympathetic outflow in rats

    J. Physiol.

    (2012)
  • S. *Ando et al.

    The effects of exercise under hypoxia on cognitive function

    PLoS One

    (2013)
  • A.F.T. Arnsten

    Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • D. *Asmaro et al.

    Cognition at altitude: impairment in executive and memory processes under hypoxic conditions

    Aviat. Space Environ. Med.

    (2013)
  • A.D. Baddeley

    Working Memory

    (1986)
  • C.B. Begg et al.

    Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias

    Biometrics

    (1994)
  • A.P. Binks et al.

    Gray matter blood flow change is unevenly distributed during moderate isocapnic hypoxia in humans

    J. Appl. Physiol.

    (2008)
  • M. *Bonnon et al.

    Criteria for psychological adaptation to high-altitude hypoxia

    Percept. Mot. Skills.

    (1999)
  • M. Borenstein et al.

    Introduction to Meta-Analysis

    (2009)
  • R.K. Bruick

    Oxygen sensing in the hypoxic response pathway: regulation of the hypoxia-inducible transcription factor

    Genes Dev.

    (2003)
  • J. Chen et al.

    Stimulus-specific signaling pathways in rabbit carotid body chemoreceptors

    Neurosci

    (2000)
  • J. Conkin

    PH2O and simulated hypobaric hypoxia

    Aviat. Space Environ. Med.

    (2011)
  • C.J. Cooper

    Anatomical and physiological mechanisms of arousal with specific reference to the effects of exercise

    Ergonomics

    (1973)
  • J. Coppel et al.

    The physiological effects of hypobaric hypoxia versus normobaric hypoxia: a systematic review of crossover trials

    Ext. Physiol. Med.

    (2015)
  • J.N. Davis et al.

    The effect of hypoxia on monoamine synthesis, levels and metabolism in rat brain

    J. Neurochem.

    (1973)
  • D.M. Devilbiss et al.

    Corticotropin-releasing factor acting at the locus coeruleus disrupts thalamic and cortical sensory-evoked responses

    Neuropsychopharmacol

    (2012)
  • J.T. Erickson et al.

    Hypoxia and electrical stimulation of the carotid sinus nerve induce Fos-like immunoreactivity within catecholaminergic and serotoninergic neurons of the rat brainstem

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1994)
  • R. Faiss et al.

    Ventilation, oxidative stress, and nitric oxide in hypobaric versus normobaric hypoxia

    Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.

    (2013)
  • B. *Fowler et al.

    Acute hypoxia fails to influence two aspects of short-term memory: implications for the source of cognitive deficits

    Aviat. Space Environ. Med.

    (1994)
  • Cited by (0)

    1

    *Included in meta-analyses.

    View full text